Sunday, November 8, 2015

Detail #230: Anti-Pegative Alignment

I posted, a good while ago, a post that described the "anti-ergative" subsystem of Finnish. The post also contains an extension of this into ditransitive verbs. However, the pegative alignment that I've previously described did not enter into that post.

What would an analogous anti-pegative alignment look like, especially in a language that in the "normal" case is pegative?

Let us consider the situation when the subject is omitted:
[omitted subject] gives thing.acc person.nom
This seems like a fairly natural way of dealing with the situation - there is no case problems whatsoever. We may want to make the situation worse before we come up with something interesting. So, first we reduce the case marking a bit:
Subjects, objects and recipients are all marked by nominative, except ditransitive subjects, which are marked with the pegative.
Given the nature of, say, dechticaetiative languages, this seems like the most probable situation for a pegative alignment in a real language.

The situations we're interested in are, schematically, something like these:

  1. [omitted subject] gives thing.nom person.nom
  2. person.[nom? peg?] gives [omitted thing.nom] person.nom
  3. person.[nom? peg?] gives thing.nom [omitted recipient]

If we extend this to a Finnic-like thing, this also would include situations where the subject is not canonically marked, although not strictly speaking omitted. We can extend this even further: non-canonical objects and non-canonical recipients.

1. Omitted or non-canonical subject
It seems this situation shouldn't call for any specific thing to happen in an anti-pegative system. Maybe, just maybe, we could have the pegative marker ascend to either the direct or the indirect object. Both possibilities seem reasonable, depending on what the speakers see as the semantics of the case marker: does it a) mark the argument with the most agency or b) the argument that provides for the recipient. To put it in very clunky English rephrasings:
a) Erin gave, and Wendy received candy.
Wendy has some agency, Erin has more agency, candy has none, thus omitting Erin, Wendy is now at the top of the agency scale.

b) From Erin, Wendy was given candy.
Wendy has agency, Erin is marked as origin. Let's use an even less natural preposition here:
Of Erin, Wendy was given candy.
Wendy was given of candy.
Both of these seem somewhat compelling (albeit probably typologically speaking very improbable). Let's present some graphs:

The standard bit of the pegative system:

SUBJDOIO
nom--------------
nomnom-------
nom-------nom
pegnomnom
Possibility 1 extends the table with these rows:
SUBJDOIO
-------nom-------
--------------nom
-------pegnom
 Possibility 2 does this instead:
SUBJDOIO
-------nom-------
--------------nom
-------nompeg
We also have possibilities 1b and 2b, where we get the following tables:
Possibility 1b:
SUBJDOIO
-------peg-------
--------------nom
-------pegnom
 Possibility 2b:
SUBJDOIO
-------nom-------
--------------peg
-------nompeg
Both of these seem pretty unlikely. If we want to see some really twisted stuff we could come up with worse varieties still, though:
SUBJDOIO
nom--------------
nomnom-------
nom-------nom
pegnomnom
-------pegnom
--------------peg
-------nom-------

Or alternatively
SUBJDOIO
nom--------------
nomnom-------
nom-------nom
pegnomnom
-------nompeg
--------------nom
-------peg-------

Both of these should be pretty bad options - especially due to the three last rows probably making it impossible for someone learning their first language to draw a reasonable conclusion about the distribution of the case marker, along the way that the anti-accusative system given last in my post on anti-ergative systems.

We could of course develop the details for an anti-pegative appearing in an otherwise decthicaetiative or "normal" alignment also. Such a follow-up post will probably appear in a few days.

No comments:

Post a Comment